Students are asked to work on large sheets of paper, and are
asked to make sure they have the ability to extend these in any direction by
cutting out extra rectangles and adding them to the initial piece of paper.
Everyone is asked to close their eyes and feel their head with the hand they do
not use for drawing. Starting at the back, students are asked to think about
how the hair gives an initial textural set of information, then asked to press
harder they are asked to feel for the bone beneath. One texture replaces
another. They are not allowed to take their hand off, but once the back of the
head is explored, the top is investigated, then the front, then the sides, some
areas being touched several times as the hand makes its way between other
sources of information. Students are encouraged to feel inside noses, mouths
and ears, to think about how a pair of glasses extends out from the surface of
the head or how an earring may suddenly change the textural world.
Then a palette of marks is made, tests and try outs using
different media whereby they are trying to make marks that approximate to
touched ‘feelings’.
Finally the drawing begins, students are told to forget what
a head looks like (this is impossible but the idea is to remind them that this
drawing doesn’t need to rely on their memory of what a head looks like), and to
begin feeling again before drawing.
Students are encouraged to start at the back of the head,
reminding them that when collecting ‘touch’ information, there is no back and
front, just a continuous surface. As they start drawing they are to invent
surface information as a trace of passage. If they are feeling an ear, how does
a mark change from hair, hair over bone to the cave like entry for the finger,
then how does the feeling tone shift from open to closed surfaces? Fingers
might trace their way across the top of a head and down over the front, moving
over one eye and missing another. Alternatively a hand may be moving up from
the neck, over the chin and then move left once the mouth is met and go on
towards an ear. Fingers may of course explore the inside of the mouth and as
this is just a continuous surface, the information continues to just spread
over the paper as the student draws.
Questions such as the nature of up-ness and down-ness in a
world without sight are asked. Is this drawing now becoming more about distance
positioning? One eye is ten thumb
lengths from the other if the feeling is done around the back of the head but
only a single thumb length, separated by a nose bridge the other way. How do
you know it’s the same eye that you are coming back to? If you were feeling
someone else’s head how would the information be encoded? You ‘know’ the
fingers have reached the nose, but if you were feeling someone else’s head you
might perhaps mistake one area for another.
One thing students are asked to do is to draw a continuous
‘feel’; this is done by drawing a ‘felt’ line that stretches from the back to
the front and to the back and to the front again. Starting with perhaps the
left ear, the fingers feel over the front of the face (or back), to the right
ear, around the back to the left ear again and onwards to the right ear again.
If there isn’t enough room on the paper for the marks they can add an
extension.
Some students start trying to make the marks very textural
by adding perhaps sand or crinkled paper, they are however encouraged to try
and make the texture visual, as these are not drawings that will be read by
touch.
Finally the drawings are put up to ‘look at’.
Some students were never able to get away from a memory of a
head. They force all their marks to make an image that has one side, always the
front of the head, eyes always related to a nose in exactly the same way you
would get if you saw the head. Others can let go and you get strange spread out
images of heads where intimations of ears, noses etc are spread out over a wide
area. In the crit students are reminded of the huge size their skin would be if
it was flattened out and pinned onto the wall.
I did this at the Swarthmore once and one student really
took off, making an image with six noses, five ears and ten eyes, each feature
simply drawn as his fingers encountered it, sometimes as his hand come down
over the front of his head, and at another time as his fingers moved from the
back to the front after exploring the neck.
As an exercise in exploring synaesthesia it was I thought
quite a good one and it’s implications often re-emerged much later in the
course when students were making sculpture. Exhibitions of sculpture for the blind were
quite common at the time and there was some debate as to how the work should be
done. Was it simply good enough to let blind people touch and feel sculpture?
Should sculpture for blind people be constructed specifically as a felt experience?
My thoughts on this overlapped with things I learnt from
working with an Indian sculptor who came to work with us at college. (He had a
show in the Art Gallery and we hosted him in the ceramics area, which was then
on Cookridge Street) He told me about how his teacher made them touch and feel
significant sculptural works blindfolded. They had to search out significant
form and in particular key points of dynamic change and point them out to the
master. These moments were indicated to the students by the master once the
blindfolds were removed. Often important form shifts that had been found by
touch were not obvious to the eye.
No comments:
Post a Comment